Stumbling on happiness by Daniel Gilbert
Think you know what makes you happy.
“One cannot divine nor forecast the conditions
that will make happiness; one only stumbles upon them by chance, in a lucky
hour, at the world's end somewhere, and hold fast to the days...” - Willa
Cather, "Le Lavandou"
Prospection - The act of looking forward in time or considering the future.
The human being is the only animal that thinks
about the future. When people are asked to report how much they think about the
past, present and future, they claim to think about the future the most. One of
the main reason is merely thinking or imagining these possibilities is itself
is a source of joy. When people daydream about the future, they tend to imagine
themselves achieving and succeeding rather than fumbling or failing.
Why we worry too much about possible outcomes?
Two reasons: First, thinking about unpleasant events can minimize their impact.
The second reason why we take such pains to imagine unpleasant events in that
fear, worry and anxiety have useful roles to play in our lives. We motivate
employees, children, spouses, and pets do the right thing by dramatizing the
unpleasant consequences of doing it wrong.
Prospection can provide pleasure and prevent
pain and this is one of the reason why our brains stubbornly insist on churning
out thoughts of the future. We have large frontal lobe so that we can look into
the future, we look into the future so that we can make prediction about it, we
make predictions about it so that we can control it . People find it gratifying
to exercise control.
Subjectivity - The fact that experience is unobservable to everyone but the
person having it.
We all steer ourselves toward the futures that
we think will make us happy, but what does that word really mean? And how can
we ever hope to achieve solid, scientific answers to questions about something
as gossamer as a feeling?
The word happiness is used to indicate at least
three related things which we might roughly call emotional happiness, moral
happiness and judgmental happiness.
Emotional happiness is the feeling common to the
feelings. For example, we have when we see our new granddaughters smile for the
first time, receive word of promotion, help a wayward tourist find the art
museum, taste Belgian chocolate toward the back of our tongue, inhale the scent
of our lover’s shampoo, hear that song we used to like so much in high-school
but have not heard in years. These feelings are different, but they also have
something in common. It is an experience; it can only be approximately defined
by its antecedents and by its relation to other experiences.
The novelist Graham Greene wrote, “Hatred seem
to operate the same glands as love”. It is possible to mistake fear for lust,
apprehension for guilt, shame for anxiety.
The dissociation between awareness and experience
can cause the same sort of spookiness with regard to our emotions. Some people
seem to be keenly aware of their moods and feelings and may even have a
novelist’s gift for describing their every shade and flavor. others of us come
equipped with a somewhat more basic emotional vocabulary that much to the
chagrin of our romantic partners, consists primarily of ‘good’ or ‘not so good’
and ‘I have already told you’.
Realism - The belief that things are in reality as they appear to be in
the mind
We use our eyes to look into space and our
imagination to look into time. Just as our eyes sometimes lead us to see things
as they are not, our imaginations sometimes lead us to foresee things as they
will not be. Imagination suffers from three shortcomings that give rise to the
illusion.
Our inattention to absences influences the way
we think about the future. Just as we don’t remember every detail of a past
event so do we fail to imagine every detail of a future event?
It is difficult to escape focus of our own
attention - difficult to consider what it is we may not be considering and this
is one of the reasons why we so often mis-predict our emotional responses to
future events.
Presentism - The tendency for current experience to influence one’s views of
the past and the future.
We have already seen how brains make ample use
of the filling-in trick when they remember the past or imagine the future and
the phrase ‘filling in” suggests an image of a hole being plugged with some
sort of material. For example, when middle-aged people are asked to remember
what they thought about premarital sex, how they felt about political issues,
or how much alcohol they drink when they were in the college, their memories
are influenced by how they think feel and drink now. The tendency to fill in
the holes in our memories of the past with material from the present is esp.
powerful when it comes to remembering emotions.
Pre-feeling often allows us to predict. our
emotions better than logical thinking does. But pre-feeling has limits. How we
feel when we imagine some things is not always a good guide to how we will feel
when we see hear wear drive eat or kiss it. For example, why do you close your
eyes when you want to visualize an object or jam your fingers in your ears when
you want to remember the melody of certain song? You do these things because
your brain must see its visual and auditory cortices to execute acts of visual
and auditory imaginations and if these areas are already busy doing their
primary jobs, then they are not available for acts of imaginations
Among life’s cruelest truths is this one:
Wonderful things are esp. wonderful the first time they happen, but their
wonderfulness wanes with repetition. But human beings have discovered two
devices that allow them to combat this tendency: variety and time (frequency)
We make mistakes when we compare with the past
instead of the possible. When we do compare with the possible, we still make
mistakes. One of the most insidious things about side by side comparison is
that it leads us to pay attention to any attribute that distinguishes the
possibilities we are comparing. All of the facts about comparison mean for our
ability to imagine future feelings are: 1) value is determined by the
comparison of one thing with another, 2) there is more than one kind of
comparison we can make in any given instances and 3) we may value something
more highly when we make one kind of comparison than when we make a different
kind of comparison.
Rationalization - the act of causing something to be or to seem reasonable
Imagination has a hard time telling us how we
will think about the future when we get there. If we have trouble foreseeing
future events, then we have even more trouble foreseeing how we will see them
when they happen.
The only thing more difficult than finding a
needle in a haystack is finding a needle in a needle-stack. When an object is
surrounded by similar objects it naturally blends in and when it is surrounded
by dissimilar objects it naturally stands out.
The interesting question is how we disambiguate
them - that is how we know which of a stimulus’s many meaning to infer on a
particular occasion. research shows that context, frequency and recency are
esp. important in this regard.
Why do people regret inactions more than
actions? One reason is that the psychological immune system has a more
difficult time manufacturing positive and credible views of inaction than of
actions. If our action turned out to be wrong one, we can console ourselves by
thinking of all the things we learned from the experience. However, if our
inactions turned out to be a missing a fortune, we can’t console ourselves by
thinking of all the things we learned from the experience because there wasn’t
one.
Unexplained events have two qualities that
amplify and extend their emotional impact. First, they strike us as rare and
unusual. The second reason is that we are esp. likely to keep thinking about
them.
Explanation robs events of their emotional
impact because it makes them seem likely and allows us to stop thinking about
them.
The eye and the brain are conspirators and like
most conspiracies, theirs is negotiated behind closed doors, in the back room,
outside of our awareness
Corrigibility - Capable of being corrected, reformed or improved.
There are many good things about getting older,
but no one knows what they are.
When people are asked to name the single object
they would try to save if their home caught fire, the most common answer is ‘my
photo album’. And yet, research reveals that memory is less like a collection
of photographs than it is a like a collection of impressionist paintings
rendered by an artist who takes considerable license with his subject.
.
Imaginations have three shortcomings: First
shortcoming is its tendency to fill in and leave out without telling us. Second
shortcoming is its tendency to project the present onto the future. Third
shortcoming is its failure to recognize that things will look different once
they happen.
What makes us think we are so darned special?
Three reasons: First, even if we aren’t special, the way we know ourselves is,
We are the only people in the world whom we can know from the inside. The
second reason is that we enjoy thinking of ourselves as special. The third
reason is that we tend to overestimate everyone’s uniqueness - that is, we tend
to think of people as more different from one another than they actually are.
Dutch polymath Daniel Bernoulli suggested that
the wisdom of any decision could be calculated by multiplying the probability
that the decisions will give us what we want by the utility of getting what we
want. By utility, Bernoulli means something like goodness or pleasure.
Bernoulli correctly realized that people are
sensitive to relative rather than absolute magnitude and his formula was meant
to take this basic psychological truth into account. Without a formula for
predicting utility, we tend to do what only our species does: imagine. Our
brain have a unique structure that allows us to mentally transport ourselves
into future circumstances and then ask ourselves how it feels to be there. If
our great brains do not allow us to go surefootedly into our futures, they at
least allow us to understand what makes us stumble.
No comments:
Post a Comment